Young Children Use Physics, Not Previous Rewards, When Learning

Remarkably, children begin to emphasize information about physics over information about previous rewards from as young as seven years of age, even when these two types of information are in direct conflict. (Image: markusspiske via Pixabay)

Children as young as seven apply basic laws of physics to problem-solving, rather than learning from what has previously been rewarded, suggests new research from the University of Cambridge.

The findings of the study, based on the Aesop’s fable The Crow and the Pitcher, help solve a debate about whether children learning to use tools are genuinely learning about physical causation or are just driven by what action previously led to a treat.

Subscribe to our Newsletter!

Receive selected content straight into your inbox.

Learning about causality — about the physical rules that govern the world around us — is a crucial part of our cognitive development. From our observations and the outcome of our own actions, we build an idea — a model — of which tools are functional for particular jobs and which are not.

However, the information we receive isn’t always as straightforward as it should be. Sometimes, outside influences mean that things that should work, don’t. Similarly, sometimes things that shouldn’t work, do. Dr. Lucy Cheke, from the Department of Psychology at the University of Cambridge, says:

In this situation, explains Cheke, a learner concerned with the outcome (a “reward” learner) would learn that the inflatable hammer was the successful tool and opt to use it for later hammering.

However, a learner concerned with physical forces (a “functionality” learner) would learn that the metal hammer produced a percussive force, albeit in the wrong place, and that the inflatable hammer did not, and would therefore opt for the metal hammer.

Now, in a study published in the open access journal PLOS ONE, Dr. Cheke and colleagues investigated what kind of information children extract from situations where the relevant physical characteristics of a potential tool are observable, but often at odds with whether the use of that tool in practice achieved the desired goal.

The researchers presented children aged 4-11 with a task through which they must retrieve a floating token to earn sticker rewards. Each time, the children were presented with a container of water and a set of tools to use to raise the level.

This experiment is based on one of the most famous Aesop’s fables, where a thirsty crow drops stones into a pitcher to get to the water. In this test, some of the tools were “functional” and some “non-functional.”

Functional tools were those that, if dropped into a standard container, would sink, raising the water level and bringing the token within reach; non-functional tools were those that would not do so, for example because they floated.

However, sometimes the children used functional tools to attempt to raise the level in a leaking container — in this context, the water would never rise high enough to bring the token within reach, no matter how functional the tool used.

At other times, the children were successful in retrieving the reward despite using a non-functional tool; for example, when using a water container that self-fills through an inlet pipe, it doesn’t matter whether the tool is functional as the water is rising anyway.

After these learning sessions, the researchers presented the children with a “standard” water container and a series of choices between different tools.

From the pattern of these choices, the researchers could calculate what type of information was most influential on children’s decision-making: reward or function. Elsa Loissel, co-first author of the study explained:

“A child doesn’t have to know the precise rules of physics that allow a tool to work to have a feeling of whether or not it should work.

“So, we can look at whether a child’s decision making is guided by principles of physics without requiring them to explicitly understand the physics itself.”

Co-first author Dr. Cheke added:

Instead, the researchers showed that information about reward was never a reliable predictor of children’s choices. Instead, the influence of functionality information increased with age — by the age of seven, this was the dominant influence in their decision making.

Provided by: University of Cambridge [Note: Materials may be edited for content and length.]

Follow us on Twitter, Facebook, or Pinterest

Recommended Stories

The Longgong Waterfall.

Exploring the Enchanting Zhukeng River Trail in Chiayi, Taiwan 

Nestled in the picturesque Ruifong Scenic Area in central Taiwan’s Chiayi County, the Zhukeng River ...

A boy using a smartphone.

Excessive Screen Time Can Affect Young People’s Emotional Development

A recent Beyond Blue survey of more than 2,000 teachers identified mental ill-health and excessive ...

An example of kintsugi.

Embracing Imperfection: The Art of Kintsugi

In Japanese culture, there is a unique tradition of not concealing the flaws of broken ...

Thomas Dambo on one of his trolls.

Thomas Dambo: Meet the Danish Artist With Whimsical Sculpture Trolls Across the World

Thomas Dambo, or “The Troll Whisperer,” is a Danish visionary artist and storyteller known for ...

A sad young woman.

Overcoming Depression: A Holistic Approach

Depression, often referred to as the “common cold of the mind,” can be a challenging ...

Giant trolls.

Thomas Dambo: The Artist Who Creates Giant Trolls Out of Trash

Thomas Dambo from Denmark has created something unique for the people of Copenhagen to enjoy ...

art-gallery

Preparing Your Art for Gallery Submissions: A Step-by-Step Guide

Ralph Waldo Emerson said: “Every artist was first an amateur.” Aspiring artists often experience excitement ...

A woman holding her hand over her face.

18 Resourceful Ways That Help Reduce Mental Stress

Many people have experienced stress at some point in their life. People generally sense that ...

Various colorful superfoods such as acai powder, turmeric, matcha green tea, quinoa, pumpkin seeds, blueberries, dried goji berries, cape gooseberries, raw cocoa, hemp seeds, and others in bowls on a marble surface with blocks spelling the words "super foods."

The Top 8 Superfoods

“Superfoods” are foods considered to be beneficial to our health. Among the superfoods that made ...

Send this to a friend